Self-Recognition of Mirror Reflection
Certainty styling is being phased out topic by topic.
Hover over keys for definitions:Determining whether an animal species has the capacity to recognize its self in a mirror can give us evidence as to whether or not that animal species has the capacity to mentally represent its self as well as other members of its species and use those representations in its mental activity. The basic approach begins by simply giving mirror exposure to the animal. Through this exposure, animals capable of self-recognition undergo similar behavioral trajectories:
1. Social behavior - responding to image as if it is another animal
2. Exploratory behavior - looking behind the mirror
3. Contingency training - testing to see if visual information matches motor behavior, like moving hands while watching the mirror
4. Self-directed behavior and self-inspection - to examine eyes, teeth, hair, genitals, etc
The final test, the Mirror Self-Recognition (MSR) Test, involves placing odorless spots on the animal that can only be visually accessed through the use of a mirror. A control spot, odorless and colorless, is also placed on the animal. If that animal approaches the mirror and consistently and systematically interacts with the visual dot more than it did before the marking process, then it evidence that the animal is capable of recognizing itself in the mirror.
The Mirror Self-Recognition Test has been done on a wide range of primates demonstrating that this ability is typically present in apes but absent in monkeys.
Below is a list of species where at least one member of that species has passed: Passing - Humans - Chimpanzees - Orangutans - Human-raised Gorillas - Cotton-top tamarins, Saguinus oedipus Self-directed behavior - Gibbons - Gorillas - Bonobos - Rhesus monkeys Not passing - Capuchin monkeys - Spider monkeys - Baboons, Papio hamadryas - Mandrills, Mandrillus sphinx - Stumptailed macaques, Macaca arctoides - Rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta - Long-tailed macaques, Macaca fascicularis - Liontailed macaques, Macaca silenus
Humans start to recognize own image on the mirror at around 2 years old (15-24 months). There are cross-cultural differences to the development of MSR. Infants that experience more face to face contigency interaction with their parents develop mirror self-recognition earlier, demonstrating that developmental activities alter this ability. This is probably why human-raised primates often show higher MSR capacities. Autistic children, when compared with typically developing children at the same developmental level, seem to vocalize less and touch their bodies more in interaction with a mirror. There is a strong correlation between object permanence tasks and self-awareness tasks. Autistic children that did poorly in the mirror self-recognition task also did poorly in an object permanence task. While the Mirror Self-Recognition Test can provide possible evidence for which primates have theory of mind capabilities, there is evidence that a number of other species also have this capability. These species tend to be very social. Bottlenose dolphins, elephants, and magpies have passed the test.
References
-
Cultural Variations in Children’s Mirror Self-Recognition, , Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 08/2011, Volume 42, Issue 6, p.1018-1029, (2011)
-
Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) do recognize themselves in the mirror: implications for the evolution of self-recognition., , PLoS One, Volume 5, Issue 9, (2010)
-
The evolution of primate visual self-recognition: evidence of absence in lesser apes., , Proc Biol Sci, 05/2009, Volume 276, Issue 1662, p.1671-7, (2009)
-
Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): evidence of self-recognition., , PLoS Biol, 08/2008, Volume 6, Issue 8, p.e202, (2008)
-
Self-recognition and self-awareness in lowland gorillas, , Self-awareness in animals and humans: Developmental perspectives , p.273-290, (2006)
-
Self-recognition in an Asian elephant., , Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2006 Nov 7, Volume 103, Issue 45, p.17053-7, (2006)
-
Parenting styles and the development of the categorical self: A longitudinal study on mirror self-recognition in Cameroonian Nso and German families, , International Journal of Behavioral Development, Volume 29, p.496-504, (2005)
-
Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence., , Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2001 May 8, Volume 98, Issue 10, p.5937-42, (2001)
-
Observations on the behavior of gibbons (Hylobates leucogenys, H. gabriellae, and H. lar) in the presence of mirrors., , J Comp Psychol, 09/2000, Volume 114, Issue 3, p.253-62, (2000)
-
A critical review of methodology and interpretation of mirror self-recognition research in nonhuman primates., , Anim Behav, 09/1999, Volume 58, Issue 3, p.459-468, (1999)
-
Comparative aspects of mirror self-recognition in great apes. , , The mentalities of gorillas and orangutans: Comparative perspectives , p.283-294, (1999)
-
Incipient mirror self-recognition in zoo gorillas and chimpanzees, , Self-awareness in animals and humans, (1994)
-
The responses of bonobos (Pan paniscus) to their mirror images: Evidence of selfrecognition, , Human Evolution, Volume 9, Issue 4, p.273 - 279, (1994)
-
Responses of capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) to different conditions of mirror-image stimulation, , Primates, Volume 30, Issue 4, p.581 - 587, (1989)
-
Self-recognition in autistic children., , J Autism Dev Disord, 12/1984, Volume 14, Issue 4, p.383-94, (1984)
-
Self-recognition in chimpanzees and orangutans, but not gorillas, , 02/1981, Volume 10, Issue 2, p.175 - 188, (1981)
-
Development of self-recognition in the infant, , Developmental Psychology, Volume 14, Issue 1, p.44, (1978)
-
Absence of self-recognition in a monkey (Macaca fascicularis) following prolonged exposure to a mirror., , Dev Psychobiol, 05/1977, Volume 10, Issue 3, p.281-4, (1977)
-
Mirror self-image reactions before age two., , Dev Psychobiol, Volume 5, Issue 4, p.297-305, (1972)
-
Chimpanzees: self-recognition., , Science, 1970 Jan 2, Volume 167, Issue 3914, p.86-7, (1970)